This page contains the text that was read by Carol Steen. To see the slides of the presentation click here.
Profiles of
Color Synesthesia
The NeCoSyn Method
Crétien van Campen & Clara Froger
This is a cooperative project by an artist and a scientist. Clara and I were both studying synesthesia when we met, about five years ago. She was
experimenting with the colors of music, taste, and odors among other senses. I was testing
self-reported synesthetes. Clara was one of the synesthetes who was tested and she
criticized the concept of color that was used and so started a long critical interaction
that has resulted in a method that we are both very pleased to have presented here today.
We apologize for
not being physically present. Actually we are somewhere on the other side of the planet as
is indicated on the picture of the globe on the slide. We live in the Netherlands: Clara
is in the world's largest harbour city Rotterdam and Crétien is in the medieval town of
Utrecht. We are very grateful to Carol Steen that she is willing to present our paper. We
are very glad that our ideas are being communicated here today .
In this
presentation, we would like to show you the NeCoSyn method, which stands for the
Netherlands Color Synesthesia method. It has been designed to capture the rich and various
synesthetic experiences in individual profiles. Reading the books and papers and
self-reports of synesthetes, the synesthetic phenomenon seems to have many aspects,
varieties, and types. Many synesthetes report having several types of synesthesia. Every
synesthete seems unique yet on closer observation there are similarities between
synesthetes that start to become apparent.
Can we compare
the experiences of synesthetes? Can we show synesthetes how their experiences relate to
the experiences of other synesthetes? And if so, in what terms?
Measuring
synesthesia in:
several types (of synesthesia)
dimensions of color experiences
The easiest
way to compare personal characteristics is to measure those characteristics. Our object
was to develop a method to measure synesthesia and take into account :
a) the several
types of synesthesia (e.g. word-color, music-color,
b) the dimensions of color experiences
(hue, saturation, brightness)
(note: these color concepts will be
explained later)
Two questions
were central:
Who is a synesthete?
What are the dimensions of synesthesia?
To be or not to be a synesthete
Consistency test
The first
question we had to deal with was: Who is a synesthete? And who is not?
Determining or
diagnosing the presence of synesthesia is a complex matter because synesthesia is not a
readily observable characteristic in people. Diagnosis has to rely on self-reports and on
psychological testing. The purpose of diagnosis and testing is to distinguish people with
synesthetic experiences from people without them. In the last two decades several
methods have been
developed to determine, diagnose, assess and distinguish synesthetes and their synesthetic
experiences.
The consistency
test is the most often used method by scientists. In short, the test distinguishes
synesthetes from non-synesthetes by measuring the strength of their word-color
correspondences over time. This test was developed by Simon Baron-Cohen and his colleagues
in the 1980's. For our method, we translated the English version into Dutch. The first
Dutch version
consisted of 50 words, containing words from four semantic categories (animals, towns,
objects, professions), letters of the alphabet, days of the week, nonsense words, and
abstract words. Later we improved and shortened the list to 25 words, including abstract
words, numbers and days of the week.
Types of
synesthesia
On the basis of
this table you may think we have covered only a small part of population of synesthetes,
but in fact we have covered the majority because the types we have included are the most
often reported ones.
Color
dimensions (part II)
Another way
we approached dimensions of color synesthesia was to look at the dimensions of the
(evoked) color experiences. When we normally speak of color we refer to hue or color tone.
In fact two other elements make up the colors that we see:
1) Brightness i.e. the amount of blackness in a color (e.g.
adding black to a red hue makes it brown), and
2) Saturation is
the purity of a color (e.g. candy red is highly saturated and pastel red, often called
pink, is much less saturated).
We chose the
Natural Color System to distinguish these three dimensions of color, Hue, Brightness and
Saturation. The Natural Color System is a logical color system built on how humans see
color. This system is based on color perceptions and differs in this respect from most
other color systems that are based on the mixing of paint or pigments. The notations are
less technical and therefore more understandable and usable for laymen.
In this slide you
see NCS colour space and one section. The color space represents all the possible colors
and consists of three dimensions:
The horizontal circle is built on the four basic hues: yellow, red, blue and green.
The vertical axis adds brightness
(lightness) or blackness to the hue (black-white), and
The horizontal axis indicates the amount of saturation from no saturation
As an example the slide shows one
cross-section of the color space (note: unfortunately the projection of the colors is not
ideal): The color purple at the right angle is a hue between red and blue. Going up the
color changes into lighter purple, almost grey-blue. Going down the color changes into
darker purple, almost black. Going to the center of the space the purple gets less
saturated (not clearly visible in this slide).
The test
We invited
self-reported synesthetes and asked them to choose color chips that fit best with their
color experiences or associations with words, music, tastes and odors. In this slide you
see a picture taken during the experiment with odor-colors. Participating synesthetes (on
the left) smell odors with their eyes shut and then choose corresponding colors from the
selection of color chips on the table. Afterwards, the assistants (on the right) note the
numbers (on the back the color chips) that contain NCS
information of hue, saturation and brightness. One of the experimentators (in the back)
keeps watch.
Materials: selection of colors
What
materials did we use in our study? First, Clara Froger, our expert on color, selected a
representative selection of 118 color chips out of the hundreds of color chips in the NCS
standard. In this slide you can see the palette of colors from which the participating
synesthetes could choose.
As stimuli to
evoke color experiences we presented the participants with:
a) a selection of 25 words. The
adaptation of the English consistency test (mentioned earlier).
b) a selection of tape recordings of
11 musical instruments chosen from a variety of wind and stringed instruments. We had
taped musical pieces (in the key of D) played by members of the Rotterdam Philharmonic
Orchestra (who had great fun doing it and wondered what this syn-thing was all about).
c) a selection of 11 tastes, a wide range of tastes including the dimensions sweet, sour,
bitter and salty. We consulted a cook.
d) a selection of 11 odors, a wide range of odors. We consulted professional odor experts.
Tasting colors
See how the
participants tasted the selected flavours (with their eyes shut) and then chose their
colors. (Note: drinking water was given to our "poor guinea pigs",between two
stimuli .
NeCoSyn profile of a synesthete
This slide
shows an example of a NeCoSyn profile of one of the synesthetes who participated in our
study. Before she participated in the study, this synesthete reported that she experiences
words in color and that she uses colors while cooking. The profile provides some
additional interesting information and details.
First, we will
explain how to read the profile. The NeCoSyn profile consists of four types of synesthesia
(see vertical axis)
the yellow-blue bar represents hue,
the reddish bar saturation and the light-dark bar brightness).
The length of each bar indicates the
strength of synesthesia, categorized into weak, moderate, and strong (see horizontal axis
on top).
The NeCoSyn
profile provided a detailed insight in her color synesthesia. Her color-word synesthesia
is very strong on all dimensions of color. Her color-taste synesthesia (that she uses for
cooking) is based on very strong dimensions of saturation and brightness of color-taste.
The hue dimension is weaker. i.e. she experiences tastes mostly in the dark-lightness scale and less in specific
hues or color tones.
Applications
in art
- practical tool for artists
- input for musical visualizers (multimedia)
- material for art education
In general
the NeCoSyn profiles are maps of someone's individual color synesthesia. Since they are
scaled, the profiles can be compared to profiles of other synesthetes and group means. We
are now looking for possible applications of this method in the fields of the arts and
sciences.
In the fields of
the arts, the NeCoSyn profiles can be used as an analytic tool by individual artists for
understanding and gaining more detailed insight in their personal synesthetic experiences.
Compare for instance how synesthetic artists like Carol Steen and David Hockney have
profited from more insight in their synesthetic experiences.
The results of
the NeCoSyn method can be used as an input for the creative process. Fred Collopy and
Harley Gittleman at this conference have shown how knowledge of synesthetic
correspondences is put into musical visualizers and other multimedia instruments.
In art schools,
art educators can use the NeCoSyn method and profiles to teach students about their
personal perceptual functioning and sensibilities. In the process of developing our method
we tested a group of more than 200 art students. The testing revealed synesthesia to some
students (made them aware of their synesthetic abilities) and stimulated discussions of
synesthesia and sensibility in others.
Applications
in science
- supplement to consistency tests
- comparison with subjective
reports and brain imaging results
- new light on incidence of synesthesia?
For scientific disciplines, e.g. medical and social
sciences, the NeCoSyn method provides a
supplement to existing consistency methods. The main advantages over the color-word test
of Baron-Cohen et al. are the assessment on color dimensions and the extensions to other
types of color synesthesia.
In comparison
with the method of self-report by synesthetes we found in our interviews with participants
that self-reports are not always in line with the test results. Another point is that the
subjective intuition of participants was objectified and specified by the NeCoSyn
instrument. In particular the extra information on color dimensions of synesthetic
experiences was new to the participants. In comparison with brain imaging, the NeCoSyn is
far less expensive to administer, less of a burden on participants and can therefore be
used for larger scale group testing . One of our subjects was tested in the medical lab of
Utrecht University (Aleman et al. 2002). In future research it will be interesting to
compare the results of brain imaging with the NeCoSyn profiles of synesthetes.
The incidence or
frequency of synesthesia still is a big question mark in synesthesia research. To
outsiders the frequency or incidence rates of
1:20 (Galton) 1:200 (Hubbard & Ramachandran), 1:2000 (Baron-Cohen cs) and 1:20,000
(Cytowic) might raise the question : What is synesthesia research about? The incidence has
to do with the number 2 but we don't know how
As a step aside,
we have the impression that our results may shed new light on determining the frequency of
synesthesia. Current research into the frequency of color synesthesia is focused on color
tone or hue. In the four types of synesthesia we measured, the mean scores for brightness
and saturation were higher than the scores on hue.
When we counted
the frequency of synesthetes according to the existing consistency test (only hue) and to
NeCoSyn methods we found frequencies for saturation and brightness were almost double the
frequency of the existing test for hue or color tone. We concluded that frequency differs
with the color dimension you look at. This is not as strange as it seems if we remember
that many people experience the sound of a bass as dark but few people experience it as
yellow or purple. This distinction between hue on the one side and brightness and
saturation on the other side might have a link with the distinction Martino & Marks
have made between strong and weak synesthesia. We are investigating this with other
scientists in the Netherlands at the moment.
Conclusion
- a practical method for artists
and scientists
- extension to other types of synesthesia
- art and science of synesthesia
To conclude, the NeCoSyn method offers a practical system
that can be applied by both artists and scientists. In our present study we restricted
ourselves to four types of color synesthesia, but the NeCoSyn method can be applied to
more sensory domains. We give the example of assessing color-tactile synesthesia. Take
little black sacs containing substances with different textures inside. Let people choose
colors after touching the textures inside. One can vary with cold and warm textures, fine
versus coarse, etcetera.
That means that
the NeCoSyn method is not finished. The artistic and scientific study of synesthesia has
grown along separate lines, more by tradition than by logic. The NeCoSyn is an example
that critical interaction of both disciplines can deepen our insight into synesthesia and
probably multisensory sensibilities in general.
More info: http://synesthesie.nl
For questions and remarks
mail to: cretien@worldonline.nl or c.froger.bv@worldonline.nl
We thank you
for attention and hope you will respond to our method.
More info is on the website
http://synesthesie.nl
(note the last e not a).
The site contains an online report of our study in English.
Though not physically present today, we are eager to hear your questions and remarks by
e-mail